This file is indexed.

/usr/share/psychtoolbox-3/PsychStairCase/MinExpEntStair.m is in psychtoolbox-3-common 3.0.11.20140816.dfsg1-1.

This file is owned by root:root, with mode 0o644.

The actual contents of the file can be viewed below.

  1
  2
  3
  4
  5
  6
  7
  8
  9
 10
 11
 12
 13
 14
 15
 16
 17
 18
 19
 20
 21
 22
 23
 24
 25
 26
 27
 28
 29
 30
 31
 32
 33
 34
 35
 36
 37
 38
 39
 40
 41
 42
 43
 44
 45
 46
 47
 48
 49
 50
 51
 52
 53
 54
 55
 56
 57
 58
 59
 60
 61
 62
 63
 64
 65
 66
 67
 68
 69
 70
 71
 72
 73
 74
 75
 76
 77
 78
 79
 80
 81
 82
 83
 84
 85
 86
 87
 88
 89
 90
 91
 92
 93
 94
 95
 96
 97
 98
 99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
function fhndl = MinExpEntStair(mode)
% Minimum Expected Entropy Staircase
%
% Caution: Currently only works with Matlab, not with GNU/Octave!
%
% The staircase gives suggestions for which probe value to test next,
% choosing the probe that will provide the most information (based on the
% principle of minimum entropy = maximally unambiguous probability
% distribution). Probes are chosen from a set of possible probe values
% provided at staircase init, and their use is evaluated based on the
% expected amount of information gain given a space of PSE and slope values
% to test over.
%
% By default, a psychometric function ranging from 0% to 100% is used, as
% is suitable for discrimination experiments with a standard in the middle
% of the possible stimulus parameter range. For other paradigms, such as
% n-AFC detection tasks, one can set the guessrate input during staircase
% init to 1/num_alternatives, e.g. .5 when doing a 2IFC detection task.
% This guess rate is thus not the rate at which participants guess instead
% of do your task (thats the lapse rate), it the minimum rate of correct
% responses as determined by your design. NB: below discussion is based on
% the default psychometric function with the full range, but all points are
% equally valid for a scaled psychometric function.
%
% It is recommended to have the staircase determine the optimal next probe
% based on only a random subset of the response history (see options
% 'toggle_use_resp_subset' and 'toggle_use_resp_subset_prop'). This makes
% its operation more robust for response errors and also avoids probe
% oscillations when the fit estimate is converging.
% When we are close to convergence, probes will tend to be near the 25% and
% 75% points. If a probe is 25% and you answer '1' (pedestal faster, which
% is likely, because it's near the correct 25% point), then for the next
% trial the peak in expected entropy reduction will generally be the 75%
% point, and vice versa. This can lead to undesirable probe sequences where
% the correct response alternates 0,1,0,1,0,1. If you choose a random
% subset, this will completely eliminate the problem. If the staircase has
% converged to where there are two almost equal expected entropy minima,
% then small variations due to the selection of subsets will randomly vary
% which minimum emerges as lowest.
% This strategy does not significantly affect optimal operation of the
% staircase. Lots of probe values provide useful information. Therefore, it
% is not crucial to have a highly accurate estimate of likelihoods, so
% relatively few trials are sufficient (less than are needed to for final
% estimates of PSE and DL). Throwing out trials for the staircase
% computation yields robustness without much cost.
%
% Another option would be to load a non-uniform prior on the space of
% possible location/mean/PSE and dispersion/slope parameters (known as mu
% and sigma respectively for a cumulative Gaussian - see option
% 'loadprior'). Probe sampling will then stay reasonable in early trials
% even if there were a couple bad responses. But this strategy is not as
% robust as using a random subset -- bad trials will continue to have an
% effect throughout.
%
% In absence of anything to base the optimal probe value on, the first
% probe is chosen randomly from the set of possible probes. When a prior
% was loaded, a likelihood distribution is available based on which the
% optional probe value can be computed. If for any other reason choosing
% the next probe based on the measure of minimum expected entropy fails,
% the staircase will fall back on the same random probe sampling strategy.
% There is an option to set the first probe value to be tested, which, for
% the first trial only, will overrule both of the above probe choice
% strategies. This can be useful if you want to be sure that the first
% trial is an easy one so the participant knows what to expect.
%
% Another measure for robustness is to choose a small lapse rate. If lapse
% would be zero and a response error is made by the observer, immediately a
% whole range of mean-slope combinations becomes impossible. If lapse rate
% is non-zero, these would still have a non-zero probability and the
% staircase can rebound. Therefore a lapse rate of 5% or even more
% depending on task difficulty is always recommended. NB: in the default
% discrimination setup of the staircase (guessrate is not specified or set
% to 0), half of the lapse rate is taken off the bottom of the psychometric
% function and half is taken off the top. So if the lapse rate is 0.05, the
% psychometric function will range from 0.025 to 0.975. In the setup for a
% n-AFC detection experiment when the psychometric function has a lower
% bound of 1/num_alternatives, the lapse rate is taken off the top. So when
% the guess_rate is set to .5 (2AFC) and the pase rate is set to .05, the
% psychometric function will range from 0.05 to 0.95.
% Note that the staircase does not support a 0 lapse rate in the first
% place as it works with log-probability and we get in trouble if we would
% take the log of a 0 probability. Any lapse rate lower than 1e-10 will be
% adjusted to 1e-10 upon calling the 'init' function.
%
% If the staircase gets stuck at one of the bounds of the probe set, check
% that the sign of the slope space matches the expected sign of the
% response. E.g., lets look at an experiment in which you are doing 2IFC
% task in which the observer is asked to report which interval contained
% the faster motion. If the observer choses the test over the pedestal
% interval the response is 1, if the observer chosen the pedestal to be
% faster, the response is 0. All slopes in the set would in this case be
% positive as the low end of the probe space (slow speeds) is associated
% with response 0 and the high end with response 1. If we however asked the
% observer to indicate the slower interval, the slopes in our slope set
% would not match the task, and the staircase would get stuck at one of the
% probe bounds. In this case, the lower end of the probe space is
% associated with the response 1 and the higher end with the response
% 0--we'd thus have a negative slope for the fitted cumulative probability
% function.
%
% The staircase currently only supports logistic and cumulative Gaussian
% (default) psychometric functions (see 'set_psychometric_func'), but
% others could easily be implemented. Changes should be needed only to the
% function "fit_a_point" at the bottom of this mfile, providing that the
% function is characterized by two parameters (which do not necessarily
% have to be PSE and slope, though that is the terminology here.
% Should you implement such a function, please do send me your code at
% dcnieho @at@ gmail.com.
%
% The above discussion assumes that response inputs to 'process_resp' are
% either 0 or 1 (see note above about their meaning) though in practice
% anything larger than 0 is treated as 1 and anything lower than 0,
% including 0, is treated as 0. the staircase can thus easily be integrated
% with programs that use a 1, -1 response scheme.
%
% For actual offline fitting of your data, you would probably want to use a
% dedicated toolbox such as Prins, N & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2009) Palamedes:
% Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data.
% http://www.palamedestoolbox.org. instead of using the function parameters
% or PSE and DL returned from staircase functions 'get_fit' and
% 'get_PSE_DL'.
% Also note that while the staircase runs far more robust when a small
% lapse rate is assumed, it is common to either fit the psychometric
% function without a lapse rate, or otherwise with the lapse rate as a free
% parameter (possibily varying only over subjects, but not over conditions
% within each subject).
%
%
% References:
%  Based on the Minimum expected entropy staircase method developed by:
%  Saunders JA & Backus BT (2006). Perception of surface slant from
%    oriented textures. Journal of Vision 6(9), article 3
%
%  Discussions of conceptually similar staircases can be found in:
%  Kontsevich LL & Tyler CW (1999). Bayesian adaptive estimation of
%    psychometric slope and threshold. Vision Res 39(16), pp. 2729-37
%  Lesmes LA, Lu ZL, Baek J & Albright TD (2010). Bayesian adaptive
%    estimation of the contrast sensitivity function: The quick CSF method.
%    Journal of Vision 10(3), article 17
%
%
% USE:
% Calling this function creates a staircase instance. The interface of the
% staircase is accessed through the returned function handle. You can
% create as many instances as you like by calling this function, each
% instance has its own internal memory/history. In that sense this is
% really OO (I'm not happy with MATLAB's OO features and also want to be
% compatible with old versions, hence the below paradigm).
% When interacting with the staircase through the function handle, the
% first argument is a string that identifies the action you want to perform
% (you can think of this as the string containing the name of the member
% function to be called) and optionally any other arguments that are needed
% for the call. See MESDemo for an example and the comments below for use
% of the different staircase functions.

% Copyright (c) 2011 by DC Niehorster and JA Saunders

% The demo and MinExpEntStair use nested functions internally, something
% not supported by Octave, so this is a no-go unless somebody rewrites this
% stuff:
if IsOctave
    error('Sorry, this function does not yet work on GNU/Octave.');
end

% private member variables
probeset    = [];           % possible probe values to be tested
aset        = [];           % pse's tested (and fitted)
bset        = [];           % slopes fitted
agrid       = [];
bgrid       = [];
lapse_rate  = [];           % lapse/mistake rate
guess_rate  = [];           % guess rate
phist       = [];           % probe history
rhist       = double([]);   % response history (0 or 1)
loglik      = [];
lik         = [];
g0          = [];
g1          = [];
% likelihood lookup table
qUseLookup  = [];           % can explicitly be set to true or false by user with 
likLookup   = [];
qLookupCompressed = false;  % lots of overlap between likelihoods for different probe values, compute and store in a format making use of this overlap

% option: use a subset of all data for choosing the next probe, default values:
quse_subset = false;        % use limited subset for computing next probe? Limited subset by discarding a fixed number of trials
quse_subset_perc = false;   % same as above, but instead use a percentage of the available data
minsetsize  = 10;           % minimum size to start subsetting
subsetsize  = 3;            % subset contains subsetsize less datapoints than full dataset
percsetsize = .8;           % percentage of data in set used

% option: set the value to test if probe history is empty
first_value = [];           % first value to test instead of random or by prior

% psychometric function that is used (default)
psychofunc     = [];
psychofuncStr  = 'cumGauss';

% subfunction
if nargin<1 || strcmpi(mode,'legacy')
    fhndl = @MinExpEntStair_internal;
    external_funs     = {@init, @loadhistory, @loadprior, @toggle_use_resp_subset, @toggle_use_resp_subset_prop, @set_first_value, @set_use_lookup_table, @get_use_lookup_table, @set_psychometric_func, @get_psychometric_func, @get_next_probe, @process_resp, @get_history, @get_fit, @get_PSE_DL};
    external_funs_str = cellfun(@(x) strrep(func2str(x),[mfilename '/'],''),external_funs,'uni',false);
elseif strcmpi(mode,'v2')
    % setup function handles
    fhndl.init                          = @init;
    fhndl.loadhistory                   = @loadhistory;
    fhndl.loadprior                     = @loadprior;
    fhndl.toggle_use_resp_subset        = @toggle_use_resp_subset;
    fhndl.toggle_use_resp_subset_prop   = @toggle_use_resp_subset_prop;
    fhndl.set_first_value               = @set_first_value;
    fhndl.set_use_lookup_table          = @set_use_lookup_table;
    fhndl.get_use_lookup_table          = @get_use_lookup_table;
    fhndl.set_psychometric_func         = @set_psychometric_func;
    fhndl.get_psychometric_func         = @get_psychometric_func;
    fhndl.get_next_probe                = @get_next_probe;
    fhndl.process_resp                  = @process_resp;
    fhndl.get_history                   = @get_history;
    fhndl.get_fit                       = @get_fit;
    fhndl.get_PSE_DL                    = @get_PSE_DL;
    
end

% public interface
    function [varargout] = MinExpEntStair_internal(mode,varargin)
        % get internal function to run
        qFun = strcmp(mode,external_funs_str);
        if any(qFun)
            % run function
            [varargout{1:nargout}] = external_funs{qFun}(varargin{:});
        else
            error('MinExpEntStair: mode "%s" unknown',mode);
        end
    end

    % init
    function [] = init(probeset_,meanset,slopeset,lapse_rate_,guess_rate_)
        probeset            = probeset_;
        aset                = meanset;
        bset                = slopeset;
        [agrid,bgrid]       = meshgrid(aset,bset);
        % init with uniform probability, normalized
        loglik              = zeros(size(agrid)) - log(numel(agrid));
        lik                 = ones(size(agrid))./numel(agrid);
        % lapse rate and guess rate
        lapse_rate          = lapse_rate_;
        % the lapse rate cannot be exactly 0 as the computed
        % probability must not be exactly 0 so we can work with
        % log(prob) without trouble, so set it to 1e-10 at least.
        lapse_rate          = max(lapse_rate,1e-10);
        % guess rate is optional, if not specified we assume a 2IFC
        % discrimination experiment where the guess rate is
        % irrelevant as function goes from always one option at the
        % one end to always the other option at the other end.
        if nargin<5
            guess_rate = 0;
        else
            guess_rate = guess_rate_;
        end

        % lapse rate:
        % 1. for a discrimination setup (guess_rate==0) the
        % lapserate basically means that instead of ranging from 0
        % to 1, the psychometric function ranges from lapse_rate/2
        % to 1-lapse_rate/2
        % 2. for a detection setup, the lower bound is guess_rate
        % and the upper bound is 1-lapse_rate

        % lower bound of pyschometric function
        % and
        % range of pyschometric function
        if guess_rate==0
            g0 = lapse_rate/2;
            g1 = 1 - lapse_rate;
        else
            g0 = guess_rate;
            g1 = 1 - lapse_rate - guess_rate;
        end
        
        set_psychometric_func(psychofuncStr);   % calls precomputeLikelihoods()
    end
                
                
    %%% load bunch of previously run trials (need probes and
    %%% responses)
    function [] = loadhistory(probes,responses)
        phist               = probes;
        rhist               = responses;

        % refit likelihood up to this point
        [loglik,lik]        = fit_all(phist,rhist);
    end
                
                
                
    %%% load a prior likelihood, so that first probe is not chosen
    %%% randomly and you can influence evolution of the fit
    function [] = loadprior(priorlik_)
        assert(all(loglik(:)==-log(numel(agrid))),'Cannot load prior if we have a likelihood already'); % this tests if it is not default inited

        priorlik = priorlik_;
        assert(size(priorlik,1)==length(bset),'Number of rows in prior much match length of slope set')
        assert(size(priorlik,2)==length(aset),'Number of columns in prior much match length of mean set')
        assert(all(priorlik(:)>=0),'Loaded prior is not expected to be a log likelihood (that is: all your probabilities should be larger than or equal to 0!)');

        loglik  = normalize_loglik(log(priorlik));
        lik     = exp(loglik);
    end
                
                
    %%% use subset of data for computing next probe
    function [varargout] = toggle_use_resp_subset(minsetsize_,subsetsize_)
        % option: extract a probe and response subset for choosing
        % the next probe, and fit just those
        % when lots of trials ran, entropy function often has two
        % local minima, with their relative values switch back and
        % forth. This will lead to large oscillations in the probe
        % value being tested (one trial a probe from the beginning
        % of set, next trial a probe from the end and the from
        % beginning of set again).
        % We want to avoid these oscillations in probe values,
        % therefore we select a limited subset of data to calculate
        % the best next probe.
        quse_subset  = ~quse_subset;
        assert(~(quse_subset && quse_subset_perc));
        if nargin>0 % change defaults
            minsetsize = minsetsize_;
            subsetsize = subsetsize_;
        end
        varargout{1} = quse_subset;
        varargout{2} = minsetsize;
        varargout{3} = subsetsize;
    end
            
                
    %%% use subset of data for computing next probe
    function [varargout] = toggle_use_resp_subset_prop(minsetsize_,percsetsize_)
        % same as above, but now always use a proportion of the
        % available data
        quse_subset_perc  = ~quse_subset_perc;
        assert(~(quse_subset_perc && quse_subset));
        if nargin>0 % change defaults
            minsetsize  = minsetsize_;
            percsetsize = percsetsize_;
        end
        varargout{1} = quse_subset_perc;
        varargout{2} = minsetsize;
        varargout{3} = percsetsize;
    end
                
                
    %%% set the first value to test. Normally the first is chosen
    %%% randomly or by using the prior that you loaded. If you prefer
    %%% to start at a fixed value, use this.
    function [] = set_first_value(first_value_)
        first_value = first_value_;
        if ~isempty(phist)
            warning('the first trial has already been run. Setting the first value now is pointless and it''ll be ignored');
        end
    end


    %%% if set to true or false, for (not) using of a precomputed lookup
    %%% table instead of evaluating the psychometric function all the time.
    %%% call this before calling init as lookup table computation is
    %%% triggered at end of init
    function [] = set_use_lookup_table(qUseLookup_)
        qUseLookup = qUseLookup_;
        if qUseLookup && isempty(likLookup)
            precomputeLikelihoods();
        end
    end
    %%% get if lookup table is currently used.
    function varargout = get_use_lookup_table()
        varargout{1} = qUseLookup;
    end
                
                
    %%% set the psychometric function to be used (default cumulative
    %%% Gaussian). Can be called at any time (but it will refit all
    %%% the data already present and thus remove the effect of any
    %%% priors).
    function [] = set_psychometric_func(funcID)
        % currently supported:
        %  'cumGauss' - Cumulative Gaussian
        %  'logistic' - logistic function
        switch funcID
            case 'cumGauss'
                psychofunc = @(x,a,b) normcdf((x-a)./b);
                
                %        1  [             x - a      ]
                %   P = --- [ 1 + erf( ----------- ) ],
                %        2  [           b*sqrt(2)    ]
                % where a and b are known as the mean (mu) and the standard
                % deviation (sigma)
                % http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Normal_distribution
                
            case 'logistic'
                psychofunc = @(x,a,b) 1./(1+exp(-(x-a)./b));
                
                %               1
                % P =   ------------------,
                %              -(x - a)/b 
                %        1 + e^
                %
                % where a and b are known as the mean (mu) and b is
                % proportional to the standard deviation (s)
                % http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Logistic_distribution
                
            otherwise
                error('Psychometric function "%s" not supported',funcID);
        end
        psychofuncStr = funcID;
        % recompute lookup table
        precomputeLikelihoods();
        % if there's any data already, refit it using the new
        % psychometric func. This would remove the effect of any
        % priors!
        if ~isempty(phist)
            ndata = min(length(phist),length(rhist));
            [loglik,lik]    = fit_all(phist(1:ndata),rhist(1:ndata));
        end
    end
    %%% get the psychometric function that is currently used.
    function [varargout] = get_psychometric_func()
        % currently possible outputs:
        %  'cumGauss' - Cumulative Gaussian
        %  'logistic' - logistic function
        varargout{1} = psychofuncStr;
    end
                
                
    %%% given history, get which probe is best to test next
    function [p,entexp,indmin] = get_next_probe()
        if isempty(phist) && ~isempty(first_value)
           % first trial and user requested a specific probe value to be tested
           p                = first_value;
           [entexp,indmin]  = deal([]);
        else
            [p,entexp,indmin]   = getnextprobe;
            if isempty(p) || isscalar(unique(loglik))
                % if we couldn't compute expected entropy, or we have a
                % uniform likelihood on which calculation was based
                % (useless prior info, such as default inited), fall
                % back on random probe selection
                p                   = probeset(round(RandLim(1,1,length(probeset))));
                [entexp,indmin]     = deal([]);
            end
        end
        phist           = [phist p];
    end
            
                
    %%% fit likelihoods for new response
    function [] = process_resp(resp) % resp on current trial
        rhist(end+1)    = resp;
        [loglik,lik]    = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,phist(end),rhist(end));
    end
                
                
    %%% retrieve probe and response history
    function [varargout] = get_history()
        varargout{1}    = phist;
        varargout{2}    = rhist;
    end
                
            
    %%% get fitted a (PSE) and b (slope) parameters and loglik.
    %%% This returns the fit of all data, also when subsetting is
    %%% enabled.
    function [varargout] = get_fit()
        kmin            = find(loglik == max(loglik(:))); % most likely combination(s) of PSE and Slope
        varargout{1}    = mean(agrid(kmin));
        varargout{2}    = mean(bgrid(kmin));
        varargout{3}    = loglik;
    end
                
            
    %%% get fitted PSE and DL (distance of 75% point from the 50%
    %%% point) and loglik. This returns the fit of all data, also
    %%% when subsetting is enabled.
    %%% This function is meant to be used for discrimination
    %%% experiments only (hence the terminology), although it will
    %%% return the inflection point and the distance between the
    %%% points that are equivalent to the 50% and 75% points after
    %%% scaling the psychometric function for all setups.
    function [varargout] = get_PSE_DL()
        [varargout{1:3}] = get_fit();
        % convert b (dispersion) parameter to DL
        switch psychofuncStr
            case 'cumGauss'
                varargout{2} = varargout{2} * erfinv(.5)*sqrt(2);
            case 'logistic'
                varargout{2} = varargout{2} * log(3);
            otherwise
                error('Psychometric function "%s" not supported',psychofuncStr);
        end
    end


% helpers (private functions, can only be called from the public
% functions above)
    function [p,entexp,indmin] = getnextprobe
        if length(rhist)>minsetsize && (quse_subset || quse_subset_perc)
            % select subset and fit
            if quse_subset_perc
                ind = NRandPerm(length(rhist),round(length(rhist)*percsetsize)); % select percentage of set
            else
                ind = NRandPerm(length(rhist),length(rhist)-subsetsize); % select set minus a few data points
            end
            [thellik,thelik] = fit_all(phist(ind),rhist(ind));
        else
            % use likelihoods already fitted for all available data
            thelik  = lik;
            thellik = loglik;
        end
        
        entexp  = zeros(1,length(probeset));
        for ksamp = 1:length(probeset)
            % p values for each possible model
            % these are used in multiple steps
            pvalsamp    = fit_a_point(probeset(ksamp),1);
            
            % expected value is sum, weighted by lik
            pval        = sum(pvalsamp(:).*thelik(:));
            
            % two possibilities for next response, 0 or 1
            % each would make a diff new likelihood function
            newloglik0  = thellik(:) + log(1 - pvalsamp(:));
            newloglik1  = thellik(:) + log(    pvalsamp(:));
            
            % important! need to normalize
            newloglik0  = normalize_loglik(newloglik0);
            newloglik1  = normalize_loglik(newloglik1);
            
            % 0 and 1 for next response each has an entropy
            ent0        = sum(-exp(newloglik0).*newloglik0);
            ent1        = sum(-exp(newloglik1).*newloglik1);
            
            % probability pval of 0, probability (1-pval) of 1
            % use these to get expected value of entropy
            entexp(ksamp)  = ent0*(1-pval) + ent1*pval;
        end
        
        indmin      = find(entexp == min(entexp),1);
        p           = probeset(indmin);        
    end

    function [loglik,lik] = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,probe,resp)
        % get likelihood of current point
        currlik = fit_a_point(probe,resp);
        % multiply with previous likelihoods
        loglik  = loglik + log(currlik);
        
        % normalize
        loglik  = normalize_loglik(loglik);
        lik     = exp(loglik);
    end

    function [loglik,lik] = fit_all(probes,resps)
        
        if length(probes) ~= length(resps)
            error('Number of probe values and responses does not match');
        end
        
        if strcmp(psychofuncStr,'cumGauss')
            % we have a fast one for this!
            loglik = FitCumGauss_MES(probes,resps,aset,bset,lapse_rate,guess_rate);
        else
            
            loglik = zeros(size(agrid));
            for p=1:length(probes)
                loglik = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,probes(p),resps(p));
            end
        end
        
        % normalize
        loglik  = normalize_loglik(loglik);
        lik     = exp(loglik);
    end

    function pval = fit_a_point(probe,resp)
        if qUseLookup
            qProbe = probeset==probe;
            if qLookupCompressed
                pval = likLookup(:,[(end-length(aset)+1):end]-find(qProbe)+1);
            else
                pval = likLookup(:,:,qProbe);
            end
        else
            pval = evalLikelihood(probe);
        end
        % if response was wrong flip probs
        if resp <= 0
            pval = 1-pval;
        end
    end
        
    function [] = precomputeLikelihoods()
        if isempty(aset)
            % called before init, parameter space not known yet, nothing to
            % do here
            return;
        end
        if ~isempty(qUseLookup) && ~qUseLookup
            % were not using lookup tables by users request, return
            return;
        end
        
        % determine if we want to precompute
        % first see if compressed format is possible. It is if same
        % stepsize for probeset and aset, as there is then significant
        % overlap between the pvalues for each probe level (could extend
        % this to one being multiples of the other...)
        stepP = mean(diff(probeset));
        stepA = mean(diff(aset));
        qLookupCompressed = abs(stepP-stepA)<=2*eps;
        
        % use lookup if compressed possible, or if table would be small,
        % or if user asked for it.
        if  (isempty(qUseLookup) && (...
                qLookupCompressed || ...                    % same stepsize for probeset and aset
                numel(agrid)*length(probeset)/128/1024<3)...% small lookup table (by some arbitrary standard of what is small, which in this case is less than 3 mb)
                ) ||...
            (~isempty(qUseLookup) && qUseLookup)            % user asked for it
            
            qUseLookup = true;
            
            nProbe = length(probeset);
            if qLookupCompressed
                [tempAGrid,tempBGrid] = meshgrid(linspace(probeset(1)-aset(1,end),probeset(end)-aset(1,1),length(aset)+length(probeset)-1),bset);
                likLookup = g0 + g1*psychofunc(0,tempAGrid,tempBGrid);
            else
                likLookup = zeros([size(agrid) nProbe]);
                for p=1:nProbe
                    likLookup(:,:,p) = evalLikelihood(probeset(p));
                end
            end
        else
            qUseLookup = false;
        end
        
    end

    function pval = evalLikelihood(probe)
        % evaluate psychometric function, incorporate lapse rate and guess rate
        pval = g0 + g1*psychofunc(probe,agrid,bgrid);
    end

    function loglik  = normalize_loglik(loglik)
        loglik  = loglik - log(sum(exp(loglik(:))));
    end
end