/usr/share/psychtoolbox-3/PsychStairCase/MinExpEntStair.m is in psychtoolbox-3-common 3.0.11.20131230.dfsg1-1build1.
This file is owned by root:root, with mode 0o644.
The actual contents of the file can be viewed below.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 | function fhndl = MinExpEntStair(mode)
% Minimum Expected Entropy Staircase
%
% Caution: Currently only works with Matlab, not with GNU/Octave!
%
% The staircase gives suggestions for which probe value to test next,
% choosing the probe that will provide the most information (based on the
% principle of minimum entropy = maximally unambiguous probability
% distribution). Probes are chosen from a set of possible probe values
% provided at staircase init, and their use is evaluated based on the
% expected amount of information gain given a space of PSE and slope values
% to test over.
%
% By default, a psychometric function ranging from 0% to 100% is used, as
% is suitable for discrimination experiments with a standard in the middle
% of the possible stimulus parameter range. For other paradigms, such as
% n-AFC detection tasks, one can set the guessrate input during staircase
% init to 1/num_alternatives, e.g. .5 when doing a 2IFC detection task.
% This guess rate is thus not the rate at which participants guess instead
% of do your task (thats the lapse rate), it the minimum rate of correct
% responses as determined by your design. NB: below discussion is based on
% the default psychometric function with the full range, but all points are
% equally valid for a scaled psychometric function.
%
% It is recommended to have the staircase determine the optimal next probe
% based on only a random subset of the response history (see options
% 'toggle_use_resp_subset' and 'toggle_use_resp_subset_prop'). This makes
% its operation more robust for response errors and also avoids probe
% oscillations when the fit estimate is converging.
% When we are close to convergence, probes will tend to be near the 25% and
% 75% points. If a probe is 25% and you answer '1' (pedestal faster, which
% is likely, because it's near the correct 25% point), then for the next
% trial the peak in expected entropy reduction will generally be the 75%
% point, and vice versa. This can lead to undesirable probe sequences where
% the correct response alternates 0,1,0,1,0,1. If you choose a random
% subset, this will completely eliminate the problem. If the staircase has
% converged to where there are two almost equal expected entropy minima,
% then small variations due to the selection of subsets will randomly vary
% which minimum emerges as lowest.
% This strategy does not significantly affect optimal operation of the
% staircase. Lots of probe values provide useful information. Therefore, it
% is not crucial to have a highly accurate estimate of likelihoods, so
% relatively few trials are sufficient (less than are needed to for final
% estimates of PSE and DL). Throwing out trials for the staircase
% computation yields robustness without much cost.
%
% Another option would be to load a non-uniform prior on the space of
% possible location/mean/PSE and dispersion/slope parameters (known as mu
% and sigma respectively for a cumulative Gaussian - see option
% 'loadprior'). Probe sampling will then stay reasonable in early trials
% even if there were a couple bad responses. But this strategy is not as
% robust as using a random subset -- bad trials will continue to have an
% effect throughout.
%
% In absence of anything to base the optimal probe value on, the first
% probe is chosen randomly from the set of possible probes. When a prior
% was loaded, a likelihood distribution is available based on which the
% optional probe value can be computed. If for any other reason choosing
% the next probe based on the measure of minimum expected entropy fails,
% the staircase will fall back on the same random probe sampling strategy.
% There is an option to set the first probe value to be tested, which, for
% the first trial only, will overrule both of the above probe choice
% strategies. This can be useful if you want to be sure that the first
% trial is an easy one so the participant knows what to expect.
%
% Another measure for robustness is to choose a small lapse rate. If lapse
% would be zero and a response error is made by the observer, immediately a
% whole range of mean-slope combinations becomes impossible. If lapse rate
% is non-zero, these would still have a non-zero probability and the
% staircase can rebound. Therefore a lapse rate of 5% or even more
% depending on task difficulty is always recommended. NB: in the default
% discrimination setup of the staircase (guessrate is not specified or set
% to 0), half of the lapse rate is taken off the bottom of the psychometric
% function and half is taken off the top. So if the lapse rate is 0.05, the
% psychometric function will range from 0.025 to 0.975. In the setup for a
% n-AFC detection experiment when the psychometric function has a lower
% bound of 1/num_alternatives, the lapse rate is taken off the top. So when
% the guess_rate is set to .5 (2AFC) and the pase rate is set to .05, the
% psychometric function will range from 0.05 to 0.95.
% Note that the staircase does not support a 0 lapse rate in the first
% place as it works with log-probability and we get in trouble if we would
% take the log of a 0 probability. Any lapse rate lower than 1e-10 will be
% adjusted to 1e-10 upon calling the 'init' function.
%
% If the staircase gets stuck at one of the bounds of the probe set, check
% that the sign of the slope space matches the expected sign of the
% response. E.g., lets look at an experiment in which you are doing 2IFC
% task in which the observer is asked to report which interval contained
% the faster motion. If the observer choses the test over the pedestal
% interval the response is 1, if the observer chosen the pedestal to be
% faster, the response is 0. All slopes in the set would in this case be
% positive as the low end of the probe space (slow speeds) is associated
% with response 0 and the high end with response 1. If we however asked the
% observer to indicate the slower interval, the slopes in our slope set
% would not match the task, and the staircase would get stuck at one of the
% probe bounds. In this case, the lower end of the probe space is
% associated with the response 1 and the higher end with the response
% 0--we'd thus have a negative slope for the fitted cumulative probability
% function.
%
% The staircase currently only supports logistic and cumulative Gaussian
% (default) psychometric functions (see 'set_psychometric_func'), but
% others could easily be implemented. Changes should be needed only to the
% function "fit_a_point" at the bottom of this mfile, providing that the
% function is characterized by two parameters (which do not necessarily
% have to be PSE and slope, though that is the terminology here.
% Should you implement such a function, please do send me your code at
% dcnieho @at@ gmail.com.
%
% The above discussion assumes that response inputs to 'process_resp' are
% either 0 or 1 (see note above about their meaning) though in practice
% anything larger than 0 is treated as 1 and anything lower than 0,
% including 0, is treated as 0. the staircase can thus easily be integrated
% with programs that use a 1, -1 response scheme.
%
% For actual offline fitting of your data, you would probably want to use a
% dedicated toolbox such as Prins, N & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2009) Palamedes:
% Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data.
% http://www.palamedestoolbox.org. instead of using the function parameters
% or PSE and DL returned from staircase functions 'get_fit' and
% 'get_PSE_DL'.
% Also note that while the staircase runs far more robust when a small
% lapse rate is assumed, it is common to either fit the psychometric
% function without a lapse rate, or otherwise with the lapse rate as a free
% parameter (possibily varying only over subjects, but not over conditions
% within each subject).
%
%
% References:
% Based on the Minimum expected entropy staircase method developed by:
% Saunders JA & Backus BT (2006). Perception of surface slant from
% oriented textures. Journal of Vision 6(9), article 3
%
% Discussions of conceptually similar staircases can be found in:
% Kontsevich LL & Tyler CW (1999). Bayesian adaptive estimation of
% psychometric slope and threshold. Vision Res 39(16), pp. 2729-37
% Lesmes LA, Lu ZL, Baek J & Albright TD (2010). Bayesian adaptive
% estimation of the contrast sensitivity function: The quick CSF method.
% Journal of Vision 10(3), article 17
%
%
% USE:
% Calling this function creates a staircase instance. The interface of the
% staircase is accessed through the returned function handle. You can
% create as many instances as you like by calling this function, each
% instance has its own internal memory/history. In that sense this is
% really OO (I'm not happy with MATLAB's OO features and also want to be
% compatible with old versions, hence the below paradigm).
% When interacting with the staircase through the function handle, the
% first argument is a string that identifies the action you want to perform
% (you can think of this as the string containing the name of the member
% function to be called) and optionally any other arguments that are needed
% for the call. See MESDemo for an example and the comments below for use
% of the different staircase functions.
% Copyright (c) 2011 by DC Niehorster and JA Saunders
% The demo and MinExpEntStair use nested functions internally, something
% not supported by Octave, so this is a no-go unless somebody rewrites this
% stuff:
if IsOctave
error('Sorry, this function does not yet work on GNU/Octave.');
end
% private member variables
probeset = []; % possible probe values to be tested
aset = []; % pse's tested (and fitted)
bset = []; % slopes fitted
agrid = [];
bgrid = [];
lapse_rate = []; % lapse/mistake rate
guess_rate = []; % guess rate
phist = []; % probe history
rhist = double([]); % response history (0 or 1)
loglik = [];
lik = [];
g0 = [];
g1 = [];
% likelihood lookup table
qUseLookup = []; % can explicitly be set to true or false by user with
likLookup = [];
qLookupCompressed = false; % lots of overlap between likelihoods for different probe values, compute and store in a format making use of this overlap
% option: use a subset of all data for choosing the next probe, default values:
quse_subset = false; % use limited subset for computing next probe? Limited subset by discarding a fixed number of trials
quse_subset_perc = false; % same as above, but instead use a percentage of the available data
minsetsize = 10; % minimum size to start subsetting
subsetsize = 3; % subset contains subsetsize less datapoints than full dataset
percsetsize = .8; % percentage of data in set used
% option: set the value to test if probe history is empty
first_value = []; % first value to test instead of random or by prior
% psychometric function that is used (default)
psychofunc = [];
psychofuncStr = 'cumGauss';
% subfunction
if nargin<1 || strcmpi(mode,'legacy')
fhndl = @MinExpEntStair_internal;
external_funs = {@init, @loadhistory, @loadprior, @toggle_use_resp_subset, @toggle_use_resp_subset_prop, @set_first_value, @set_use_lookup_table, @get_use_lookup_table, @set_psychometric_func, @get_psychometric_func, @get_next_probe, @process_resp, @get_history, @get_fit, @get_PSE_DL};
external_funs_str = cellfun(@(x) strrep(func2str(x),[mfilename '/'],''),external_funs,'uni',false);
elseif strcmpi(mode,'v2')
% setup function handles
fhndl.init = @init;
fhndl.loadhistory = @loadhistory;
fhndl.loadprior = @loadprior;
fhndl.toggle_use_resp_subset = @toggle_use_resp_subset;
fhndl.toggle_use_resp_subset_prop = @toggle_use_resp_subset_prop;
fhndl.set_first_value = @set_first_value;
fhndl.set_use_lookup_table = @set_use_lookup_table;
fhndl.get_use_lookup_table = @get_use_lookup_table;
fhndl.set_psychometric_func = @set_psychometric_func;
fhndl.get_psychometric_func = @get_psychometric_func;
fhndl.get_next_probe = @get_next_probe;
fhndl.process_resp = @process_resp;
fhndl.get_history = @get_history;
fhndl.get_fit = @get_fit;
fhndl.get_PSE_DL = @get_PSE_DL;
end
% public interface
function [varargout] = MinExpEntStair_internal(mode,varargin)
% get internal function to run
qFun = strcmp(mode,external_funs_str);
if any(qFun)
% run function
[varargout{1:nargout}] = external_funs{qFun}(varargin{:});
else
error('MinExpEntStair: mode "%s" unknown',mode);
end
end
% init
function [] = init(probeset_,meanset,slopeset,lapse_rate_,guess_rate_)
probeset = probeset_;
aset = meanset;
bset = slopeset;
[agrid,bgrid] = meshgrid(aset,bset);
% init with uniform probability, normalized
loglik = zeros(size(agrid)) - log(numel(agrid));
lik = ones(size(agrid))./numel(agrid);
% lapse rate and guess rate
lapse_rate = lapse_rate_;
% the lapse rate cannot be exactly 0 as the computed
% probability must not be exactly 0 so we can work with
% log(prob) without trouble, so set it to 1e-10 at least.
lapse_rate = max(lapse_rate,1e-10);
% guess rate is optional, if not specified we assume a 2IFC
% discrimination experiment where the guess rate is
% irrelevant as function goes from always one option at the
% one end to always the other option at the other end.
if nargin<5
guess_rate = 0;
else
guess_rate = guess_rate_;
end
% lapse rate:
% 1. for a discrimination setup (guess_rate==0) the
% lapserate basically means that instead of ranging from 0
% to 1, the psychometric function ranges from lapse_rate/2
% to 1-lapse_rate/2
% 2. for a detection setup, the lower bound is guess_rate
% and the upper bound is 1-lapse_rate
% lower bound of pyschometric function
% and
% range of pyschometric function
if guess_rate==0
g0 = lapse_rate/2;
g1 = 1 - lapse_rate;
else
g0 = guess_rate;
g1 = 1 - lapse_rate - guess_rate;
end
set_psychometric_func(psychofuncStr); % calls precomputeLikelihoods()
end
%%% load bunch of previously run trials (need probes and
%%% responses)
function [] = loadhistory(probes,responses)
phist = probes;
rhist = responses;
% refit likelihood up to this point
[loglik,lik] = fit_all(phist,rhist);
end
%%% load a prior likelihood, so that first probe is not chosen
%%% randomly and you can influence evolution of the fit
function [] = loadprior(priorlik_)
assert(all(loglik(:)==-log(numel(agrid))),'Cannot load prior if we have a likelihood already'); % this tests if it is not default inited
priorlik = priorlik_;
assert(size(priorlik,1)==length(bset),'Number of rows in prior much match length of slope set')
assert(size(priorlik,2)==length(aset),'Number of columns in prior much match length of mean set')
assert(all(priorlik(:)>=0),'Loaded prior is not expected to be a log likelihood (that is: all your probabilities should be larger than or equal to 0!)');
loglik = normalize_loglik(log(priorlik));
lik = exp(loglik);
end
%%% use subset of data for computing next probe
function [varargout] = toggle_use_resp_subset(minsetsize_,subsetsize_)
% option: extract a probe and response subset for choosing
% the next probe, and fit just those
% when lots of trials ran, entropy function often has two
% local minima, with their relative values switch back and
% forth. This will lead to large oscillations in the probe
% value being tested (one trial a probe from the beginning
% of set, next trial a probe from the end and the from
% beginning of set again).
% We want to avoid these oscillations in probe values,
% therefore we select a limited subset of data to calculate
% the best next probe.
quse_subset = ~quse_subset;
assert(~(quse_subset && quse_subset_perc));
if nargin>0 % change defaults
minsetsize = minsetsize_;
subsetsize = subsetsize_;
end
varargout{1} = quse_subset;
varargout{2} = minsetsize;
varargout{3} = subsetsize;
end
%%% use subset of data for computing next probe
function [varargout] = toggle_use_resp_subset_prop(minsetsize_,percsetsize_)
% same as above, but now always use a proportion of the
% available data
quse_subset_perc = ~quse_subset_perc;
assert(~(quse_subset_perc && quse_subset));
if nargin>0 % change defaults
minsetsize = minsetsize_;
percsetsize = percsetsize_;
end
varargout{1} = quse_subset_perc;
varargout{2} = minsetsize;
varargout{3} = percsetsize;
end
%%% set the first value to test. Normally the first is chosen
%%% randomly or by using the prior that you loaded. If you prefer
%%% to start at a fixed value, use this.
function [] = set_first_value(first_value_)
first_value = first_value_;
if ~isempty(phist)
warning('the first trial has already been run. Setting the first value now is pointless and it''ll be ignored');
end
end
%%% if set to true or false, for (not) using of a precomputed lookup
%%% table instead of evaluating the psychometric function all the time.
%%% call this before calling init as lookup table computation is
%%% triggered at end of init
function [] = set_use_lookup_table(qUseLookup_)
qUseLookup = qUseLookup_;
if qUseLookup && isempty(likLookup)
precomputeLikelihoods();
end
end
%%% get if lookup table is currently used.
function varargout = get_use_lookup_table()
varargout{1} = qUseLookup;
end
%%% set the psychometric function to be used (default cumulative
%%% Gaussian). Can be called at any time (but it will refit all
%%% the data already present and thus remove the effect of any
%%% priors).
function [] = set_psychometric_func(funcID)
% currently supported:
% 'cumGauss' - Cumulative Gaussian
% 'logistic' - logistic function
switch funcID
case 'cumGauss'
psychofunc = @(x,a,b) normcdf((x-a)./b);
% 1 [ x - a ]
% P = --- [ 1 + erf( ----------- ) ],
% 2 [ b*sqrt(2) ]
% where a and b are known as the mean (mu) and the standard
% deviation (sigma)
% http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Normal_distribution
case 'logistic'
psychofunc = @(x,a,b) 1./(1+exp(-(x-a)./b));
% 1
% P = ------------------,
% -(x - a)/b
% 1 + e^
%
% where a and b are known as the mean (mu) and b is
% proportional to the standard deviation (s)
% http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Logistic_distribution
otherwise
error('Psychometric function "%s" not supported',funcID);
end
psychofuncStr = funcID;
% recompute lookup table
precomputeLikelihoods();
% if there's any data already, refit it using the new
% psychometric func. This would remove the effect of any
% priors!
if ~isempty(phist)
ndata = min(length(phist),length(rhist));
[loglik,lik] = fit_all(phist(1:ndata),rhist(1:ndata));
end
end
%%% get the psychometric function that is currently used.
function [varargout] = get_psychometric_func()
% currently possible outputs:
% 'cumGauss' - Cumulative Gaussian
% 'logistic' - logistic function
varargout{1} = psychofuncStr;
end
%%% given history, get which probe is best to test next
function [p,entexp,indmin] = get_next_probe()
if isempty(phist) && ~isempty(first_value)
% first trial and user requested a specific probe value to be tested
p = first_value;
[entexp,indmin] = deal([]);
else
[p,entexp,indmin] = getnextprobe;
if isempty(p) || isscalar(unique(loglik))
% if we couldn't compute expected entropy, or we have a
% uniform likelihood on which calculation was based
% (useless prior info, such as default inited), fall
% back on random probe selection
p = probeset(round(RandLim(1,1,length(probeset))));
[entexp,indmin] = deal([]);
end
end
phist = [phist p];
end
%%% fit likelihoods for new response
function [] = process_resp(resp) % resp on current trial
rhist(end+1) = resp;
[loglik,lik] = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,phist(end),rhist(end));
end
%%% retrieve probe and response history
function [varargout] = get_history()
varargout{1} = phist;
varargout{2} = rhist;
end
%%% get fitted a (PSE) and b (slope) parameters and loglik.
%%% This returns the fit of all data, also when subsetting is
%%% enabled.
function [varargout] = get_fit()
kmin = find(loglik == max(loglik(:))); % most likely combination(s) of PSE and Slope
varargout{1} = mean(agrid(kmin));
varargout{2} = mean(bgrid(kmin));
varargout{3} = loglik;
end
%%% get fitted PSE and DL (distance of 75% point from the 50%
%%% point) and loglik. This returns the fit of all data, also
%%% when subsetting is enabled.
%%% This function is meant to be used for discrimination
%%% experiments only (hence the terminology), although it will
%%% return the inflection point and the distance between the
%%% points that are equivalent to the 50% and 75% points after
%%% scaling the psychometric function for all setups.
function [varargout] = get_PSE_DL()
[varargout{1:3}] = get_fit();
% convert b (dispersion) parameter to DL
switch psychofuncStr
case 'cumGauss'
varargout{2} = varargout{2} * erfinv(.5)*sqrt(2);
case 'logistic'
varargout{2} = varargout{2} * log(3);
otherwise
error('Psychometric function "%s" not supported',psychofuncStr);
end
end
% helpers (private functions, can only be called from the public
% functions above)
function [p,entexp,indmin] = getnextprobe
if length(rhist)>minsetsize && (quse_subset || quse_subset_perc)
% select subset and fit
if quse_subset_perc
ind = NRandPerm(length(rhist),round(length(rhist)*percsetsize)); % select percentage of set
else
ind = NRandPerm(length(rhist),length(rhist)-subsetsize); % select set minus a few data points
end
[thellik,thelik] = fit_all(phist(ind),rhist(ind));
else
% use likelihoods already fitted for all available data
thelik = lik;
thellik = loglik;
end
entexp = zeros(1,length(probeset));
for ksamp = 1:length(probeset)
% p values for each possible model
% these are used in multiple steps
pvalsamp = fit_a_point(probeset(ksamp),1);
% expected value is sum, weighted by lik
pval = sum(pvalsamp(:).*thelik(:));
% two possibilities for next response, 0 or 1
% each would make a diff new likelihood function
newloglik0 = thellik(:) + log(1 - pvalsamp(:));
newloglik1 = thellik(:) + log( pvalsamp(:));
% important! need to normalize
newloglik0 = normalize_loglik(newloglik0);
newloglik1 = normalize_loglik(newloglik1);
% 0 and 1 for next response each has an entropy
ent0 = sum(-exp(newloglik0).*newloglik0);
ent1 = sum(-exp(newloglik1).*newloglik1);
% probability pval of 0, probability (1-pval) of 1
% use these to get expected value of entropy
entexp(ksamp) = ent0*(1-pval) + ent1*pval;
end
indmin = find(entexp == min(entexp),1);
p = probeset(indmin);
end
function [loglik,lik] = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,probe,resp)
% get likelihood of current point
currlik = fit_a_point(probe,resp);
% multiply with previous likelihoods
loglik = loglik + log(currlik);
% normalize
loglik = normalize_loglik(loglik);
lik = exp(loglik);
end
function [loglik,lik] = fit_all(probes,resps)
if length(probes) ~= length(resps)
error('Number of probe values and responses does not match');
end
if strcmp(psychofuncStr,'cumGauss')
% we have a fast one for this!
loglik = FitCumGauss_MES(probes,resps,aset,bset,lapse_rate,guess_rate);
else
loglik = zeros(size(agrid));
for p=1:length(probes)
loglik = fit_additional_data_point(loglik,probes(p),resps(p));
end
end
% normalize
loglik = normalize_loglik(loglik);
lik = exp(loglik);
end
function pval = fit_a_point(probe,resp)
if qUseLookup
qProbe = probeset==probe;
if qLookupCompressed
pval = likLookup(:,[(end-length(aset)+1):end]-find(qProbe)+1);
else
pval = likLookup(:,:,qProbe);
end
else
pval = evalLikelihood(probe);
end
% if response was wrong flip probs
if resp <= 0
pval = 1-pval;
end
end
function [] = precomputeLikelihoods()
if isempty(aset)
% called before init, parameter space not known yet, nothing to
% do here
return;
end
if ~isempty(qUseLookup) && ~qUseLookup
% were not using lookup tables by users request, return
return;
end
% determine if we want to precompute
% first see if compressed format is possible. It is if same
% stepsize for probeset and aset, as there is then significant
% overlap between the pvalues for each probe level (could extend
% this to one being multiples of the other...)
stepP = mean(diff(probeset));
stepA = mean(diff(aset));
qLookupCompressed = abs(stepP-stepA)<=2*eps;
% use lookup if compressed possible, or if table would be small,
% or if user asked for it.
if (isempty(qUseLookup) && (...
qLookupCompressed || ... % same stepsize for probeset and aset
numel(agrid)*length(probeset)/128/1024<3)...% small lookup table (by some arbitrary standard of what is small, which in this case is less than 3 mb)
) ||...
(~isempty(qUseLookup) && qUseLookup) % user asked for it
qUseLookup = true;
nProbe = length(probeset);
if qLookupCompressed
[tempAGrid,tempBGrid] = meshgrid(linspace(probeset(1)-aset(1,end),probeset(end)-aset(1,1),length(aset)+length(probeset)-1),bset);
likLookup = g0 + g1*psychofunc(0,tempAGrid,tempBGrid);
else
likLookup = zeros([size(agrid) nProbe]);
for p=1:nProbe
likLookup(:,:,p) = evalLikelihood(probeset(p));
end
end
else
qUseLookup = false;
end
end
function pval = evalLikelihood(probe)
% evaluate psychometric function, incorporate lapse rate and guess rate
pval = g0 + g1*psychofunc(probe,agrid,bgrid);
end
function loglik = normalize_loglik(loglik)
loglik = loglik - log(sum(exp(loglik(:))));
end
end
|